Hide metadata

dc.date.accessioned2018-02-23T17:08:54Z
dc.date.available2019-12-15T23:46:00Z
dc.date.created2018-01-10T15:08:38Z
dc.date.issued2017
dc.identifier.citationKvernbekk, Tone . Evidence-Based Educational Practice. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Education. 2017 Oxford University Press
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10852/60383
dc.description.abstractEvidence-based practice (EBP) is a buzzword in contemporary professional debates, for example, in education, medicine, psychiatry, and social policy. It is known as the “what works” agenda, and its focus is on the use of the best available evidence to bring about desirable results or prevent undesirable ones. We immediately see here that EBP is practical in nature, that evidence is thought to play a central role, and also that EBP is deeply causal: we intervene into an already existing practice in order to produce an output or to improve the output. If our intervention brings the results we want, we say that it “works.” How should we understand the causal nature of EBP be understood? Causality is a highly contentious issue in education, and many writers want to banish it altogether. But causation denotes a dynamic relation between factors and is indispensable if one wants to be able to plan the attainment of goals and results. A nuanced and reasonable understanding of causality is therefore necessary to EBP, and this we find in the INUS-condition approach. The nature and function of evidence is much discussed. The evidence in question is supplied by research, as a response to both political and practical demands that educational research should contribute to practice. In general, evidence speaks to the truth value of claims. In the case of EBP, the evidence emanates from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) and presumably speaks to the truth value of claims such as “if we do X, it will lead to result Y.” But what does research evidence really tell us? It is argued here that a positive RCT result will tell you that X worked where the RCT was conducted and that an RCT does not yield general results. Causality and evidence come together in the practitioner perspective. Here we shift from finding causes to using them to bring about desirable results. This puts contextual matters at center stage: will X work in this particular context? It is argued that much heterogeneous contextual evidence is required to make X relevant for new contexts. If EBP is to be a success, research evidence and contextual evidence must be brought together. This is an accepted version of a chapter originally published in Oxford Research Encyclopedias. © 2017 Oxford University Pressen_US
dc.languageEN
dc.publisherOxford University Press
dc.titleEvidence-Based Educational Practiceen_US
dc.typeChapteren_US
dc.creator.authorKvernbekk, Tone
cristin.unitcode185,18,1,0
cristin.unitnameInstitutt for pedagogikk
cristin.ispublishedtrue
cristin.fulltextpostprint
dc.identifier.cristin1539999
dc.identifier.bibliographiccitationinfo:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:book&rft.btitle=Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Education&rft.spage=&rft.date=2017
dc.identifier.pagecount2400
dc.identifier.doihttp://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190264093.013.187
dc.identifier.urnURN:NBN:no-63034
dc.type.documentBokkapittelen_US
dc.type.peerreviewedPeer reviewed
dc.source.isbn9780190264093
dc.identifier.fulltextFulltext https://www.duo.uio.no/bitstream/handle/10852/60383/4/EDU-187.pdf
dc.type.versionAcceptedVersion
cristin.btitleOxford Research Encyclopedia of Education


Files in this item

Appears in the following Collection

Hide metadata